click below
click below
Normal Size Small Size show me how
Lang Acquisition
Lecture 17 & Gleitman & Discussion 4/9
Question | Answer |
---|---|
how are clues utilized to reconstruct the underlying structure from the surface form? | using the sentence analyzing machinery which operates in terms of several general strategies & depending on how easy/hard the sentence is to analyze using the strategies, the sentence will be correspondingly easy or hard for subjects to understand |
sentence analyzing machinery (SAM) starts out with | a strong bias about what kind of sentence it is about to hear |
what order do the propositional roles of the sentence analyzing machinery appear in? | doer, act, done-to (or grammatically: subject, verb, object) |
when does the sentence analyzing machinery doer-first strategy usually fail? | for passive sentences because the done-to comes before the doer so S.A.M. has to correct and revise and reassign the first noun phrase as done-to and the second as the doer |
evidence for the sentence analyzing machinery comes from | studies of reaction time |
the first noun-did-it strategy reaction time study found that | subjects reached the decision faster when the sentence was in the active rather than the passive form |
interrupted sentences which presents propositions in the middle of another sentences require | more effort than sentences which present the propositions one at a time |
some of the best evidence for understanding how the nervous system accomplishes its tasks in the usual case comes from looking at | the rare cases where this machinery fails |
there is some evidence that the meaning of a passive sentence is recovered as quickly as the meaning of an active sentence if | the words used give strong semantic cues to the overall propositional thought |
normal children learn their native tongue to a high level of proficiency during | the preschool years |
a popular but false hypothesis about language learning is that it is based on | explicit correction or reinforcement by parents where mistakes are immediately pointed out to the young learner, who subsequently avoids them |
in actual practice mistakes in grammar and pronunciation | generally go unremarked but there are corrections of fact |
in the first year of life | true speech is absent |
the gesture-and-babble interaction helps children to | become linguistically socialized |
useable knowledge of speaking and listening requires | social interactions between more that one person at a time |
at a very early age infants make some perceptual distinctions about the sounds that they hear that are | crucial to later language learning |
infants are more attentive to hearing their caretakers language than | a foreign language by the fourth day of life |
infants initially respond to just about all sound distinctions in any language but then they discover the phonemes of their native language by | learning to ignore the distinctions that don't matter |
in the first year of an infants life they are sensitive to | every contrast that occurs in any human language but this sensitivity diminishes by 12 months of age |
syntax is | the general principles for combining the finite stock of words into infinitely many sentences |
a special speech style taken on when talking to babies is Motherese is characterized by | a special tone of voice, with high pitch, slow rate, and exaggerated intonations |
there is evidence that infants prefer | Motherese more that adult-to-adult speech even though they understand neither |
Motherese has acoustic properties that make it useful for | learning to recognize the phrases and sentence units in speech |
children begin to understand a few words that their caregivers are saying as early as | 5 to 8 months of age |
actual talking begins sometime between | 10 and 20 months of age |
an Childs early vocabulary tends to involve | things that can be moved around and manipulated or that move by themselves the child's environment |
function words and suffixes are usually late to be used by children because | they are usually not stressed and occur with low pitch in the caregiver's speech so they are uninteresting and they are grammatical items which are of little use for a child's one-word utterances |
beginners often.. | under generalize the meaning of a word while overgeneralizing the meaning of other words |
the under generalization of overgeneralization of words is common for the first | 75 or so words a child utters but very rare thereafter |
the functional approach about a child's earliest word meanings is when developmental psychologists believe | that children use words to classify things together that act alike in their world |
the featural approach about a child's earliest word meanings is when developmental psychologists believe | that children use words to designate things that look alike, that share certain perceptual features |
developmental psychologists believe a child's earliest word meanings are based on prototypes | where children name things to the extent that they resemble a particular item which serves as the model for the entire concept |
many investigators of child language believe young children have propositional ideas in mind even when | they are speaking only one word at a time |
by two years of age a child's vocabulary | increases to many hundreds of words that they start using in primitive sentences |
why are children's sentences so short? | 1. hard to learn function words have low pitch and are thus less salient in the sound wave as children perceive it 2 problem of memory and information handling that makes construction of a. complex sentence difficult |
language learners are neurologically "programmed" to expect human language to have a certain specific syntactic organization and variation | is only on a few details (or parameters) in the real languages of the world |
by two and a half years of age children progress to speaking | little sentences that contain all 3 terms of a basic proposition and function words have begun to appear |
at the age or 4 or 5 children start making errors in | their word formation and syntax (for example the use of the paste tense suffix --ed) & resist change even when they hear their parents use the correct form |
children invent causative verbs where | nouns are used as verbs to convey action meaning in sentences |
5 year olds typically have a vocabulary of | 10,000 to 15,000 words |
a caregivers speech style helps language learners to decide | which words are about the whole objects and which words are about their parts |
some ways of conceptualizing experiences are natural to humans while others are less natural thus a child can learn more easily if | they assume that each word represents some "natural" organization of experience |
young children acquire the "basic-level" words before | the superordinates or subordinates |
colors and textures are not as salient in the child's perceptual organization as | shapes, so the slightest change in will convince the learner that the new item is something wholly different from the previous item |
children are disposed to organize the world into | overarching categories- things, events, properties, etc. |
most sentences are | completely novel |
humans know over | 100,000 words |
the first job in language acquisition is to | distinguish language sounds from other sounds |
the second job in language acquisition is to parse it into phonemes and then parse phonemes into words or | chop it up into atomic units of language sound and divide sounds in to individual words |
a problem with assigning meaning to words is that | same sound can refer to different things |
children tend to correct themselves on grammar despite | little negative feedback |
the general language principles that are shared between all languages are | nouns, verbs, adjectives, etc |
the holophrastic stage of language acquisition is from 12-14 months & includes | one word utterances with no syntax, & need context (gestures, affect) to disambiguate, with under generalization & overgeneralization for the first 75 words |
in the holophrastic stage infants do understand some phrases which suggests that | their comprehension is ahead of production |
the telegraphic stage of language acquisition is from +/- 24 months and includes | 2 word utterances with the correct use of word order & can convey a log of information succinctly |
infants start learning syntactic and grammar rules in what stage of language acquisition? | after the holophrastic and telegraphic stage |
what language rules must humans learn? | must learn how words are combined (grammar) and must generalize to novel sentences so we can't just memorize wordings |
around 4-5 years old children learn.. | general rules that apply to new cases which implies that language learning is generative, not just imitation |
around 5 years old how many new words a day are children learning | around 10 new words per day |
when learning a new words meaning there is an inherent bias that | new words refer to shape |
what is the critical period effects | people who learn language after age 10-12 never acquire native ability |
around 8-10 months of age infants can | discriminate all phonemes, but eventually lose discriminations that are not apart of their own language |
the exaggerated speech in Motherese helps to | provide clearer indications of important boundaries between words, phrases, and clauses |
in Motherese the clarity and distinctness helps with | phonological contrast & distinguishing different vowel sounds |
infants prefer to listen to the voice of | a female speaking mothers over the same voice speaking adult directed speech |
when learning syntax and rules children tend to | overgeneralize syntax rules |